student engagement. However, there may be no implications from this research as Jensen (2000) has found learning style to be irrelevant in the complex process of learning. Concluding Discussion The most important finding in this study was that in these classes, cognitive style gap was detrimental to motivation in courses taught by adaptive faculty members; even more detrimental if student's cognitive style gap with the faculty member was larger than 20 points. Realizing the necessity of student motivation to foster student engagement (Astin, 1984), this relationship must be further researched. Another important finding was that in these undergraduate courses, students with a 20-point cognitive style gap or higher did not have significantly higher levels of stress than students with less than a 20-point gap with their faculty member. If this relationship is confirmed with further research, this finding limits the application of Kirton's (2003) adaption-innovation theory in the undergraduate classroom. Maybe the undergraduate classroom environment is unique in that students are more conforming to the expectations of the faculty member; more so than the group dynamics of managing change for which the KAI was developed. If the undergraduate classroom is a different environment, the threshold for cognitive style gap to cause stress in undergraduate students may be higher than 20 points. Also, the undergraduate classroom environment may not offer enough individual interaction between the faculty member and each student to constitute working together to solve a problem. This study provides evidence that dissimilar cognitive styles between students and their faculty member may contribute to student stress, student motivation and student engagement. However, results were inconclusive to why some classes found cognitive style gap contributing to these dependent variables and in other classes cognitive style