were given instructional discourse that matched their individual learning style. Results were mixed with only nine of eighteen studies supporting the contention that students learn best when taught in their preferred learning style. In another example, Wingenbach (2000) used the Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT) (Witkin, Oltman, Raskin & Karp, 1971) and found undergraduates with field-independent learning styles had a moderately positive and significant relationship with student academic achievement. However, DeTure (2004) used the GEFT and found no evidence to significantly predict student academic achievement from learning style. Why do these findings conflict? If Kirton (2003) is correct, all students are capable of achieving academic success within their own cognitive style. If academic achievement is dependent on the expectations of the faculty member, could dissimilar cognitive style between students and the faculty member be an intervening variable? Kirton's Adaption-Innovation (A-I) Theory (2003) identifies cognitive style as a preference to the amount of structure that one undertakes when solving problems. Within the continuum of learning style families (Coffield et al., 2004), Kirton's work resides in the fourth family noted above, "flexibly stable learning preferences" (p.9). Some people are more adaptive when solving problems, and other people are more innovative when solving problems. Adaptiveness and innovativeness anchor on continuums of how people approach problems, produce ideas, and adhere to structures. These three constructs measure a fixed perception of an environment in which the individual interacts with and responds in the process of learning while solving a problem; a fit to this study's definition of learning style (Claxton and Ralston, 1978). More detail will be given to Kirton's cognitive style's relationship to learning in chapter 2.