tasks; individuals' ratings across all of the child-related task items showed participants reported fathers doing more for 8% of the items, mothers doing more for 62% of the items, and equal sharing of child-related tasks for 30% of the items. It appears fathers' relative lack of involvement may lead to less positive marital outcomes, as has been established in previous research involving mothers (C. P. Cowan & Cowan, 1988; Cowan & Cowan, 2000; Hackel & Ruble, 1992; Ruble et al., 1988). It may also be that less happy marriages lead fathers to participate less in caring for their child, as has been a more typical finding for fathers reported in the existing research (Belsky & Kelly, 1994; Paley et al., 2005; Shapiro, 2005; Van Egeren, 2004). Interestingly, child-related task differentiation was identified as a significant predictor of marital disaffection in the regression analysis while violated expectations were not. (Violated expectations were correlated with disaffection, however.) Numerous other studies have found violated expectations to be more strongly associated with marital dissatisfaction than the couples' actual division of labor (Cowan & Cowan, 2000; Goldberg & Perry-Jenkins, 2004; Hackel & Ruble, 1992; Kalmuss et al., 1992; Pancer & Pratt, 2000; Ruble et al., 1988). This difference may be due to the marital outcome measure used in the present study, which assessed disaffection rather than dissatisfaction. Marital disaffection is a gradual process involving the deterioration of caring and attachment in a relationship, perhaps through the accumulation of various disappointments and conflicts (Kayser, 1996). While violated expectations may signify a disappointment in the marriage, these disappointments are only starting to accrue during the first year after the birth of the couple's child. Although violated expectations may be