input of information through the visual-motor system. Basically, a variety of eye-related muscles are involved in eye movement. Measuring eye movement involves an examination of the number of fixations, duration of fixation, and the sequence of picture elements fixated. These eye fixations are called saccades and become more articulated as a child grows. When a preschooler looks between different objects, he or she typically uses head and neck movements rather than eye movements. By age 6 or 7, the child should be able to read a book or a board with proper eye movements and without moving his or her head or body (Schieman, 2002). Due to body or neck movement, a student who has low functioning eye-movement skills may have difficulties in keeping his or her place while reading or adding numbers in columns (Scheiman, 2002). Further, reading speed may be reduced due to slow eye movements, recurrent loss of place, or due to a small span of recognition (the width of text that is processed per fixation). Therefore, we may conclude that underachieved or incorrectly developed eye movement may be associated with reading difficulties. A study conducted with a selected group of 17 children with reading disabilities and 21 without reading disabilities found that visual tasks such as eye movement and visual search are able to differentiate good and poor readers as well as phonological tests (Eden et al., 1993). Specifically, Eden and his colleagues (1995) found some visual deficits including vertical tracking and vergence control in eye movement skills were stronger predictors than some other verbal skills. Their findings strongly support the hypothesis that some children with reading difficulties have visual problems. In summary, Rayner (1995) recapitulated the process of reading and eye movements by introducing five principal issues: (a) the span of effective vision, (b) integration of information across eye movements, (c) eye movement control (where to