ability (Pearson & Fielding, 1991; Pressley, 2000), and domain knowledge ((Dole, Duffy, Roehler, & Pearson, 1991), along with differences in such dispositions as the reader's motivation, goals, and purposes, are important sources of variability in reading comprehension. Second, text is the second element affecting reading comprehension. Duke (2000) documented that children who attend schools in poor districts have many fewer texts available than do children who attend schools in richer areas; the availability of texts in homes and libraries varies similarly. Texts that treat certain social issues or that require an interpretation and appreciation of alternative perspectives may be considered inappropriate by parents from some cultural or religious groups. Text factors thus interact with reader, activity, and context in determining the difficulty of comprehension. Third, activity refers to the acts a reader engages in with a text, and it encompasses purpose, operations, and consequences. Reading comprehension can be improved with strategies when readers themselves apply across a number of different texts (National Reading Panel, 2000). For example, teachers may teach students to activate their own background knowledge, to draw inferences as they read, or to restate information in the text (Chan, Cole, & Barfett, 1987). The successful activation of two areas of reading-fluency and comprehension-completes reading. Significant Predictors of Reading Success Many studies including longitudinal studies have been conducted to identify the predictors of current and later reading ability. Such studies yielded many important predictors of RD. Currently, phonological awareness and rapid-naming variables are very frequently involved in prediction studies. Further, a critical role of phonological and naming speed factors in classifying reading disabilities have been identified by many reading studies (e.g., Morris et al., 1998). Within the field of reading-related vision