believed that these problems were related to the left hemisphere of the brain, which controls the storage of visual memory because these children seemed to have difficulty remembering names of letters and words (Clark & Uhry, 1995). In 1928, Samuel Orton, an American neurologist, theorized that people perceive visual inputs from both right and left visual fields and relay this input as mirror images to the visual cortices (Clark & Uhry, 1995). For most people, one side of brain becomes dominant over the other and suppresses the image from the non-dominant hemisphere; however, he believed some people who poorly perform 'dominance', may perceive backward images. He believed this poor dominance might produce reading problems (Clark & Uhry, 1995). Therefore, from the early 20th century, vision research related to RD has been continuously undertaken. However, researchers revealed that reversal and sequencing errors did not appear in a large proportion of people with RD, and most of people with RD showed more oral reading errors. Moreover, Vellutino (1978, 1983) indicated that poorer readers are as good as nondisabled readers on copying confusable letters and words from memory. Rather, they showed problems at naming and pronouncing the items. With these findings, he concluded that visual deficits could not explain RD. Moreover, several major reviews of the evidence concluded that visual deficits are not a major cause of reading disability (Stanovich, 1982; Velluntino, 1979). Since then, numerous studies have demonstrated that understanding the sound structure of language (phonological awareness) is a strong predictor of reading success. In contrast, studies on visual processing factors have been under heavy attack by reading researchers during the last three decades. As discussed earlier, inconsistent and conflicting findings have fueled the debate and have hindered research on visual