peripheral characteristic" (p. 633), considerable conflict persists regarding its validity. Factor analytic studies of this construct (Lindbloom & Faw, 1982) have yielded more factors than the theory accounts for on the generalized scale. Wallston, Wallston, Kaplan, and Maedes (1976) suggest that an explanation of these contradictory findings may be found in the theory. According to this theory, it is assumed that increasing an individual's experience in a given situation will lead to the development of specific expectancies. These expectancies subsequently play a greater role in determining one's future behavior in that situation than more generalized expectancies. It stands to reason that research whose aim is the prediction of behavior in specific situations could profit from the use of more specific expectancy measures. (p. 580) The authors believed that a health-related locus of control scale would provide more sensitive predictions of the relationship between locus of control and health behavior. Richard Lau (1982) conducted a study "to explore possible determinants of health locus of control beliefs" (p. 323). Using a multidimensional health locus of control battery, he found that early health habits concerning self-care, such as brushing teeth, getting exercise, getting enough sleep, proper nutrition, and other health habits regarding utilization of the medical profession were positively correlated with internal health locus of control. Recent illness was not related to either internal or external control, suggesting that health locus of control could be a stable trait. Another factor related to internality is socio-economic status, with higher SES subjects being positively correlated with internality. A surprising result of his survey was that having a physician in the family was not a significant factor in determining locus of control.