BULLETIN FLORIDA STATE MUSEUM VOL. 33(4) maintaining the boundaries of the adult female's home range. Even if young on the periphery of the adult range were effective in preventing encroachment on the adult's range by other bobcats, only a relatively small fraction of the total range perimeter would be "protected." The tendency of the young to concentrate their activity at the periphery of the natal range during the late pregnancy and postnatal period of the mother may have been due to the combination of their age and the increased aggressiveness of the mother during the breeding season. In this connection, Winegarner (1985a) once observed the semi-tame female attacking one of her older young when it returned to "her domain." There are few comparative data on movements of bobcat family groups in other parts of the geographic range. The best description is that of Bailey (1979) for Idaho bobcats. General patterns in the movements of Florida kittens were similar to those observed in Idaho in that movements were generally restricted to den areas until the kittens were past 3 months of age. After this age, more of the range was used by the offspring. In South Carolina, Marshall (1969), found that an older juvenile used the entire range of its mother, and Kitchings and Story (1979) reported the same behavior for a juvenile in eastern Tennessee. Few data were obtained on dispersal and establishment of home ranges by juveniles, but we believe that most moved out of the study area. However, one apparent case of a male (M6) and a female (F8) of the same litter becoming established as a breeding pair in the natal range was recorded. These presumed young of F3 remained in the mother's range after the deaths of the overlapping adult males (Ml, M3) and abandonment of the range by the mother. With time, both individuals extended their movements beyond the natal range, and the female had litters in spring 1980 and 1981. The northward expansion of the male's range apparently coincided with the disappearance of an unmarked male in that area and probably resulted in overlapping part or all of the range of his mother (F3), who had shifted her range into the area earlier (Figs. 4D-5D). Little information is available on dispersal and subsequent home range establishment of young bobcats in other geographic regions. Bailey (1974) in Idaho noted that young bobcats appeared to avoid settling in areas occupied by residents and recovered five tagged kittens 1-2 years later as adults from sites outside the study area, although he had no data on their movements during the intervening period. Two subadults radiotracked in South Carolina by Griffith et al. (1980) abandoned their initial activity areas (in the natal range?) in early spring and began a pattern of movements involving temporary localization of activity for varying periods of time. They concluded that their data supported a model of subadult bobcat dispersal characterized by "nomadic search for unoccupied, resource-adequate home range sites," as implied by Bailey (1974). The tendency of the subadults monitored by Griffith et al. (1980) to use