WASSMER ET AL.: SOUTH-CENTRAL FLORIDA BOBCAT ECOLOGY 203 elsewhere in the natal range was on 22 February when their tracks were found together in a region of the adult range 1 and 3 km from their respective areas. We last observed tracks of the juveniles within the mother's range in mid- April, and Winegarner (1985a) could not find their tracks in June and July. The adult gave birth to a new litter on or about 1 April, indicating that females may mate and bear young while juveniles of their previous litter are still within the natal range. Adult female F1 also was apparently still associated with one or more juveniles of her April 1981 litter in December 1981 after the birth of a new litter in mid-October, based on observations of her tracks accompanied by those of a young individual. Evidence for breeding while still associated with young of the last litter was not confined to the semi-tame female. F9 was observed on 30 November 1980 with one of her two kittens born in mid-April and gave birth to her next litter in early January 1981. Based on a 2-month gestation period (McCord and Cardoza 1982) mating probably occurred in early November. Winegarner (1985a) also monitored female F1 and her older young in 1981 (see above) by means of tracks. She believed that the mother actually abandoned the portions of her range occupied by the juveniles, then reoccupied the areas following dispersal of the young. She concluded, therefore, that dispersal of juvenile bobcats is preceded by "a period of solitude" in the natal home range and this "temporary tenure" of the young in the mother's range "insures that the home range is occupied." Although she did not elaborate as to why establishment of exclusive home ranges by young at the edge of the adult female's range is necessary to insure its occupancy, she may have assumed that breeding females during late pregnancy and early postnatal care of the young reduce their home range and that the presence of their older young at the periphery helps to hold the area against other bobcats so that the female can reoccupy it when her new litter is old enough to travel with her. Our combined radiotracking and trailing data do not support this interpretation. Although the female tended to visit the periphery of her range less frequently and for shorter periods for about 2 months after the birth of her litter in April, radio fixes regularly placed her in the area where the activity of the juveniles was concentrated during this period. In addition, by searching for her tracks in areas where she had been radio located the previous night, it was confirmed that she continued to mark her former range boundary in these areas. Although the last contact between F1 and her old juveniles, based on close proximity of adult and juvenile tracks, recorded by Winegarner (1985a) was in late January, we observed evidence of the adult and one or both young in the same area on two nights in late February. On one of these occasions the female and both young were in an area remote from the areas in which the young were usually found (see above). Our data, therefore, indicate that the young did not establish exclusive home ranges prior to dispersing from the natal area and suggest that the young probably did not play a significant role in