BALEE & MOORE: SIMILARITY AND VARIATION IN PLANT NAMES 211 five languages: Arawetd (Ar) pinuwa-'i, Asurini (As) pinuwa-'iwa, Ka'apor (K) pinuwa-'4, Temb6 (T) pinuwa-'iw, Wayapi (W) pino. By contrast, the words for 'moela de mutum' are bewilderingly different: Ar iwa-pedi, As iwa-kaw- 'iwa, K kupapa-'iran-I', T iwa-zu-'iw-ran, W mitil-'ay. This paper attempts to answer this question, at least partially, for the semantic domain of ethnobotany, by investigating similarity and variation among words for a given corpus of plant species in five different languages of Tupi-Guarani. These five languages are spoken in a broad arc in lower Amazonia. The possible factors that may a priori help explain why words for some plant species are similar while words for others vary across languages of the same family include (1) cultural ones, such as plant utility and/or management; (2) geographical ones, such as proximity and similarity of environments; (3) diachronic linguistic ones, such as borrowing and degree of genetic relatedness, as well as (4) the linguistic properties of the words used to designate plants, including their morphological or semantic structure. An investigation of such factors, to our knowledge, has never before been carried out with regard to South American languages. We have collected data on similarity and variation of words for 167 botanical species native to the neotropics in the Arawet6, Asurini do Xingu, Ka'apor, Temb6, and Wayapi languages of eastern Amazonia (Tables 1, 2, and 3). Although these data were collected initially for non-linguistic purposes, they are highly appropriate for the investigation of factors involved in similarity and variation in plant words among different languages of the same family. First, the five languages are dispersed in four linguistic sub-groupings of Tupi-Guarani (A.R. Rodrigues 1984/85; A.R. Rodrigues, pers. comm. 1988), with only Wayipi and Ka'apor being classified in the same sub-grouping. Second, these five languages are spoken in three ecologically diverse regions: the Xingu River basin of north-central Brazil for Arawet6 amd Asurini, the Gurupi/Turiaqu River basins of extreme eastern Amazonia for Ka'apor and Temb6, and the Oiapoque River basin of northern Amazonia for WayIpi. Third, although all five groups are horticultural, they exhibit notable differences in crop staples and patterns of utilization of non-domesticated species. For example, the Arawet6 rely heavily on maize, in contrast to the other groups who are more dependent on tubers; the Asurini traditionally eschewed hog plum (Spondias mombin L.), which is an esteemed edible fruit of the other groups. Fourth, collections and determinations of voucher specimens for most of the 167 species in our sample have been obtained for the five languages. Finally, the corpus of data is large enough to test statistically propositions regarding similarity and variation in plant words across the five languages. Some years ago, Brent Berlin and his colleagues (Berlin et al. 1973) published a pioneering paper on the retention of plant words in two Mayan languages. They proposed that such retention reflected the cultural