the correct philosophy is to try to limit the potential for the choice of unsuitable blocks. This can probably be achieved by setting an upper limit of 8 plots per block and requiring that the blocks be made compact. However I shall assume that the experimenter does have the opportunity to assess variation within the proposed experimental area and that (s)he, in collaboration with the farmer, can make judgements identifying those plots likely to produce similar results. To avoid the suggestion that a "Block" must be rectangular in nature, I propose to call sets of plots judged to be similar "Groups". It is crucial that this identification of groups be separate from decisions about the choice of treatments. These two aspects of design inevitably interact later but we must first try to ensure that we have the best possible grouping of plots (as well as the most relevant set of treatments. Let us consider an example from one of the on-farm experiments in the Poza Rica area. There were 24 plots in three rows of eight plots, as shown: Row 3 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 Row 2 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Row 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Row I is at the bottom of the hill, row 2 above row 1, and row three above both. The correct grouping system, identified by the experimenter, was three groups (1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8) (9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16) (17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24). The experiment was actually designed in four blocks of six plots (1,2,3,4,5,6) (17,18,19,20,21,22) (9.10,11,12,13,14) (7,8,15,16,23,24) You can probably guess how many treatments there were! The correct design would have three blocks of eight plots each with each block containing all six treatments plus extra plots for two of the treatments, the extra two being different in each block. The irony of this particular situation is that the actual allocation of treatments to blocks was inevitably equivalent to the correct design (can you see why?) and the analysis that should be applied to the results should treat the data as if the design had been intended to be the correct design (i.e. 3 blocks of eight plots, not 4 blocks of six plots). However there is one difference between the allocation of treatments to plots in the ideal design and in that actually used and this is that the randomisation of the design in three blocks would include all possible allocations of treatments to the eight plots (see design example 1, section B). Naturally experimenters cannot always expect to achieve the ideal design when trying to use an inappropriate block structure. Another example in the Poza Rica farms had 24 plots as shown: Row 5 19 20 21 22 23 24 Row 4 13 14 15 16 17 18 Row 3 7 8 9 10 11 12 Row 2 4 5 6 Row 1 1 2 3