23 situations with language attitudes of the community-at-large. They believe that presently available language proficiency tests are too narrow in scope and based on what psychologists, linguists, and educators perceive about what children should do rather than what children actually can do. The assessment of language dominance is reviewed by Burt and Dulay (1978) who stress the importance of distinguishing between naturalistic and linguistically manipulative tasks. Measurements on each of these tasks will result in different sets of linguistic in formation. Pedraza and Pousada (1980) find that their ethnographic techniques for determining language dominance revealed different information than standardized language tests or self-report measurers. They conclude that students labeled as alingual by measures used at school were quite fluent in one or two languages in their home environ ment. They emphasize that for bilinguals, labels indicating language proficiency and dominance may be unstable descriptors that change over time and within different social environments. Rodriguez-Brown (1979) finds that educational treatment, instruction in first and/or second language, is more important than language pro ficiency. Language treatment during the instructional year plays a significant role in reading achievement gains students evidence at the end of the year. In this study, Spanish-speaking primary students who entered school with different levels of English and Spanish proficiency were given different educational treatments in reading instruction. The students with moderate knowledge of English were provided instruction in English and Spanish, while students who were still Spanish dominant,